How Does the Phonology of a One Year Old Differ

Last Updated: 04 Sep 2020
Pages: 3 Views: 95
Table of contents

How does the phonology of a one-year-old differ from that of a two-three-year-old?

Describe the main changes to be expected over the first year of word use. A child between one and three years undergoes considerable development in their phonological ability. They adopt specific phonological processes and it will be explored when and how children use these to attain accurate pronunciations and how individual differences affect phonological development. Grunwell (1981) suggests that the first six months of productive language development (0. 9-1. years) is word-based, because of the limited phonetic variants and progressive changes in pronunciation. However, he suggests 1. 6-2. 0 years is the end of the first stage of speech development, which is co-occurrent with the achievement of an active vocabulary of 50 words. Menn & Vihman (2011) suggest that these early words parallel babbling, in that they are characterized by unmarked elements and structures, such as plosives, nasals, and glides; simple vowels, and CV structures. This stage of development in a child’s inventory may be characterized as a ‘proto-system’, as the child-forms do not resemble adult words.

However, the child’s early phonetic inventory (Table 1) suggests that the child has a basic contrastive system and indicates that their phonological system has commenced, which will see an increase in new words and the emergence of two-word utterances.

Order custom essay How Does the Phonology of a One Year Old Differ with free plagiarism report

feat icon 450+ experts on 30 subjects feat icon Starting from 3 hours delivery
Get Essay Help
m n p b t d w

Table 1: A phonetic inventory of a child 1. 6-2. 0 years. Grunwell (1981) presents a ‘chronology of phonological processes’ which reflects a child’s phonological development in terms of the disappearance of simplifying processes between 2. 0-4. years. These processes are summarised in table 2 and show that reduplication and consonant harmony are the only structural simplification processes outgrown by age two, which agree with the findings of Vihman & Greenlee (1987). Structural simplification is generally typical of the earlier stage of phonological development. However, phonological processes; final consonant deletion, cluster reduction, fronting, gliding, and stopping are regularly used by children until nearly age three, with less consistent use thereafter

Vihman (2004) states that half of her three-year-old subjects used gliding and palatal fronting, but the substitution of inter-dental fricatives was regularly used by all subjects and is associated with the highest frequency of errors. Table 2 suggests that velar fronting in particular is the first systemic simplification to be outgrown, at 2. 6 years. Despite this, it shows that obstruents do not occur in a child’s inventory until age three and that these must be mastered before obstruent and liquid clusters can be produced correctly. Vihman & Greenlee (1987) show that the specific phonetic tendencies found at age one seem to be unrelated to the phonological errors at age three and suggest that phonetic preferences change over time. Vihman (2004) suggests that children with an exploratory approach to phonological development explore a wide range of sounds at age one and were more likely to delete consonants at age three, whereas children with a systematic approach constrain their word selection patterns at age one and are less likely to use whole-word processes at age three. However, Vihman & Greenlee (1987) show that 73% of children’s utterances at age three were judged intelligible, which correlates with lower phonological error scores. In conclusion, individual differences are significant in one's phonological system and problematic in generalizing ‘normal’ developments. However, a three-year-old child will have overall relative phonological advance and the majority of simplifying phonological processes used at age one will no longer apply regularly.

Reference

  1. Grunwell, P. (1981).
  2. The development of Phonology: A Descriptive Profile. First Language. 2: 161-191 Ingram, D (1986).
  3. Ch10: Phonological Development: Production. In Fletcher, P & Garman, M. Language acquisition pp223-239 CUP: UK 2nd Edition Menn, L. & Vihman, M. M. (2011).
  4. Part V: Features in Phonological development: Features in Child Phonology: Inherent, Emergent, or Artefacts of Analysis? In Clements, N. G & Ridouane, R (Ed) Where Do Phonological Features Come From? Cognitive, Physical, and developmental bases of distinctive speech categories. John Benjamins Publishing Company. p259-303 Vihman, M. M (2004).
  5. Ch3: Later Phonological Development. In Bernthal, J. E & Bankson, N. W, Articulation and Phonological Disorders, pp105-138. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 5th Edition. Vihman, M. M., Ferguson, A. & Elbert, M (1986).
  6. Phonological development from babbling to speech: common tendencies and individual differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7: 3-40 Vihman, M. M. & Greenlee, M. (1987).
  7. Individual Differences in Phonological Development: Ages one and three years Journal of speech and hearing research. 30: 503-521.

Cite this Page

How Does the Phonology of a One Year Old Differ. (2016, Dec 13). Retrieved from https://phdessay.com/how-does-the-phonology-of-a-one-year-old-differ/

Don't let plagiarism ruin your grade

Run a free check or have your essay done for you

plagiarism ruin image

We use cookies to give you the best experience possible. By continuing we’ll assume you’re on board with our cookie policy

Save time and let our verified experts help you.

Hire writer